|
Food Neophobia
Last modified at 2/2/2021 3:18 PM by Maren Johnson
Page Content Food neophobia is a naturally occurring reaction in humans that
protect individuals from the risk of being poisoned by consuming
potentially harmful foods. It accounts for a person’s reluctance to
consume either new or unusual foods, based on one’s culture and current
diet (Rozin, 1997; Stallberg-white&Pliner;, 1999). Individuals may
perceive and expect how an acceptable food should look and smell. As a
consequence, an unfamiliar food that does not fall into one’s acceptable
category will be rejected (Dovey et al., 2008). However, food neophobia
may affects food choice and limits overall dietary variety especially
in children (Pliner&Melo;, 1996; Falciglia et al., 2000; Skinner et
al., 2002).
Factor effecting food neophobia
Food neophobia occurs in all type of consumers. The level of food
neophobic response varies among individuals. The level of Food neophobia
for each individual is affected by cultural, gender, age, education,
social, economics, and urbanization (Tourila et al., 2001; Flight et
al., 2003; Olabi et al., 2009).
Type of Food neophobia
Three types of consumers can be classified according to behavior of
novel food rejection. These groups are: neophilic, neutral, and
neophobic consumers. Neophilic consumers tend to accept unfamiliar
foods. In contrast, neophobic consumers tend to reject unfamiliar foods.
One way of determining which group a person is classified in uses the
Food Neophobia Scale (FNS) developed by Pliner
and Hobden (1992). That questionnaire measures one’s
agreement/disagreement on 10 statements about novel foods or eating
situations on a 7-point bipolar scale (1=strongly disagree to 7=
strongly agree).
Type of Novel Food
Tuorila (2001) classified novel foods into five categories. The
types of novel food are: (1) functional foods or medicinal food, the
foods that claimed to promote health and immunity; (2) genetically
modified foods, foods that are produced using gene technology; (3)
nutritionally modified foods, foods that have better nutritional
benefits than conventional foods; (4) organic foods, the foods are
produced or farmed in traditional conditions, and (5) ethnic foods that
are specifically familiar to one culture but unfamiliar to others.
Increasing Willingness to try novel food and Food neophobia reduction
An individual’s expectations toward food products have found to play
a critical role in consumers’ motivation to try novel foods (Tuorila et
al., 1994; Tuorila et al, 1998; Deliza&MacFie;, 1996;
Jaeger&MacFie;, 2001; Hurling&Shepherd;, 2003). Consumers’
willingness to try novel food can be increased by providing positive
experiences such as giving descriptive sensory (visual, odor, and taste
exposures) and nutritional information. Information seems to be helpful
to increase willingness to try novel foods and reduce food neophobia
(Pelchat & Pliner, 1995; McFarlane & Pliner, 1997; de Graaf et
al., 2005). Neophobia also can impact scores given to new foods in
sensory acceptance tests (Henriques et al., 2008; King et al., 2008).
Often “new” products receive lower scores than more familiar products
because of the impact of neophobia. Ideas such as providing information
on the product, serving new foods in the context of how they might be
used, and testing the new product monadically (in contrast to testing
within a set of other products) may help overcome the “penalty” often
associated with neophobia (Tourila et al., 1995; Wansink et al., 2005,
Deliz&MacFie;), but many of those idea remain undertested.
References
- De Graaf, C., Cardello, A.V., Kramer, F.M., Lesher, L.L. Meisekman,
H.L., & Scutz, H.G. (2005). A comparison between liking ratings
obtained under laboratory and field conditions: The role of choice.
Appetite, 44, 15-22.
- Deliza, R., & MacFie, H.J.H. (1996). The generation of sensory
expectation by external cues and its effect on sensory perception and
hedonic ratings: A review. Journal of Sensory Studies, 11, 103-128.
- Dovey, T.M., Staple, P.A., Gibson, E.L., & Halford. J.C.G. (2008).
Food neophobia and ‘picky/fussy’ eating in children: a review. Appetite,
50, 181-193.
- Falciglia, G.A., Couch, S.C., Gribble, L.S., Pabst, S.M., & Frank,
R. (2000). Food neophobia in childhood affects dietary variety. Journal
of the American Dietetic Association, 100, 1474-1478.
- Flight, I., Leppard, P., Cox, D.N., (2003). Food neophobia and
associations with cultural diversity and socio-economic status amongst
rural and urban Australian adolescents. Appetite, 41, 51-59.
- Henriques, A.S., King, S.C., & Meiselman, H.L. (2008). Consumer
segmentation based on food neophobia and its application to product
development. Food Quality and Preference, 20, 83-91.
- Hurling, R., & Shepherd, R. (2003) Eating with your eyes: effect of
appearance on expectations of liking. Appetite, 41, 167-174.
- Jaeger, S. & MacFie, H.J.H. (2001). The effect of advertising format
and means-end information on consumer expectations for apples. Food
Quality and Preference, 12, 189-205.
- King, S.C., Meiselman, H.L., Henriques, A. (2008). The effect of choice
and psychographics on the acceptability of novel flavors. Food Quality
and Preference, 19, 692-696.
- McFarlane, T., & Pliner, P. (1997). Increasing willingness to taste
novel foods: Effects of nutrition and taste information. Appetite, 28,
227-238.
- Olabi, A., Najm, N.O., Baghdadi, O.K., Morton, J.M. (2009). Food
neophobia levels of Lebanese and American college students. Food Quality
and Preference, 20, 353-362.
- Pelchat, M., & Pliner, P. (1995). “Try it: you’ll like it; effects
of information on willingness to try novel foods. Appetite, 24, 153-531.
- Pelchat, M. L. & Pliner, P. (1997). “Try It: It’s Good and It’s Good
for You”: Effects of Taste and Nutrition Information on Willingness to
Try Novel Foods. Appetite. 28: 89-102.
- Pliner, P. & Hobden, K. (1992). Development of a scale to measure
the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite, 19, 105-120.
- Rozin, P. 1997. The Use of Characteristic Flavoring in Human Culinary
Practice. In C. M. Apt (Ed.) Flavor: Its Chemical, Behavioral and
Commercial Aspects. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
- Skinner, J.D., Carruth, B.R., Wendy, B., & Ziegler, P.J. (2002).
Children’s food preference’s food preferences. Journal of American
Dietetic Association, 102, 1638-1647.
- Stallberg-white, C., & Pliner, P. (1999). The effect of flavor
principles on willingness to taste novel foods. Appetite, 33(2), 209-221
- Tuorila, H.,Lahteenmaki, L., Pohjalainen, L., & Lotti, L. (2001).
Food neophobia among the Finns and related responses to familiar and
unfamiliar foods. Food Quality and Preference, 12, 29-37.
- Tuorila, H., Meiselman, H.L., Bell, R., Cardello, A.V. & Johnson, W.
(1994). Role of sensory and cognitive information in the enhancement of
certainty and liking for novel and familiar foods. Appetite, 23,
231-246.
- Tuorila, H., Meiselman, H., Cardello, A., & Lesher, L. 1998. Effect
of expectations and the definition of product category on the acceptance
of unfamiliar food. Food Quality and Preference. 9(6): 421-430.
- Urala, N., & Lahteenmaki, L. (2006). Hedonic ratings and perceived
healthiness in experimental functional food choices. Appetite, 47,
302-314.
- Wansink, B., Ittersum, K., Painter, J. E., (2005). How descriptive food
names bias sensory perceptions in restaurants. Food Quality and
Preference, 16, 393-400.
|